Return to Video

Lecture 6.2: Distributing Cognition (Part 1/2) (16:44)

  • 0:03 - 0:10
    There are several different ways that interfaces can help people think more fluidly
  • 0:10 - 0:15
    by distributing their cognition into artifacts in the world.
  • 0:15 - 0:20
    When interfaces help people distribute a cognition, it can encourage experimentation;
  • 0:20 - 0:25
    it can scaffold learning and reduce errors through redundancy;
  • 0:25 - 0:28
    it can show only the differences that matter;
  • 0:28 - 0:32
    it can convert slow calculation into fast perception;
  • 0:32 - 0:36
    it can support chunking, specially by experts;
  • 0:36 - 0:38
    it can increase efficiency of interactions;
  • 0:38 - 0:41
    and it can facilitate collaboration.
  • 0:41 - 0:44
    Let’s go through these one at a time.
  • 0:46 - 0:53
    Here’s a video game that, I bet, many of you all know: This is the Tetris video game.
  • 0:53 - 1:00
    And there was a very clever study that David Kirsh and Paul Maglio at UC San Diego ran,
  • 1:00 - 1:06
    where they looked at Tetris players playing Tetris, and what they found was really interesting.
  • 1:06 - 1:11
    In Tetris, as you may know, you can use keys to move objects around on the screen.
  • 1:11 - 1:18
    And at a certain point you could hit the space bar to drop the object and try and get a row of a few blocks.
  • 1:18 - 1:22
    Moving and rotating pieces on the screen may seem like a waste of time
  • 1:22 - 1:25
    because you have a limited amount of time before the block hits the bottom.
  • 1:25 - 1:29
    So, how do you use that most efficiently?
  • 1:29 - 1:34
    It turns out that people move the block around the screen
  • 1:34 - 1:39
    more than they — in some purely theoretical sense — need to.
  • 1:39 - 1:44
    So, in essence, I’m trying out different places that the blocks could go.
  • 1:44 - 1:51
    Maybe this is just for novices that when you’re learning Tetris you need to feel things out
  • 1:51 - 1:54
    but as you become more of an expert that’s no longer the case?
  • 1:54 - 1:56
    Exactly the opposite!
  • 1:56 - 2:03
    Kirsh and Maglio found that experts actually relied more heavily on moving objects in the world.
  • 2:03 - 2:08
    And what they were doing is they were distributing the cognitive effort of “what if” scenarios —
  • 2:08 - 2:11
    What if I placed it over here? What if I placed it over there?
  • 2:11 - 2:13
    You absolutely can do that in your mind,
  • 2:13 - 2:19
    but it turns that in this case, for most people, it’s cheaper to do it out in the world
  • 2:19 - 2:24
    to be able to turn the cognitive task of reasoning about all of the what-if scenarios
  • 2:24 - 2:30
    into a perceptual task of “Oh yeah, that block would work perfectly right there.”
  • 2:30 - 2:35
    It saves you the effort of having to, in particular, mentally rotate the different pieces
  • 2:35 - 2:38
    to figure out how they would fit into the screen.
  • 2:38 - 2:44
    Here’s another example from the learning sciences: There are called Montesorri blocks.
  • 2:44 - 2:51
    And what we have here are beads that provide a physical representation for number[s].
  • 2:51 - 2:57
    Especially for young children, numbers are really abstract concepts, difficult to get your head around.
  • 2:57 - 3:04
    And these physical instantiations can help teach addition, multiplication and other simple arithmetic operations.
  • 3:04 - 3:11
    So, for example, if I’m going to take three and multiply it by three to get three squared,
  • 3:11 - 3:18
    well, I can see that I have three by three — I have a square and I can see that it’s composed of nine beads.
  • 3:18 - 3:22
    And there’re other addition and multiplication options that you could with these also.
  • 3:22 - 3:24
    And by having this redundant information —
  • 3:24 - 3:29
    by taking an abstract concept and “realfying” it and making it concrete —
  • 3:29 - 3:32
    it can help scaffold learning and reduce errors.
  • 3:32 - 3:36
    The Montessori blocks example and the Tetris example
  • 3:36 - 3:42
    show us the power of providing a visual or physical instantiation of abstract ideas.
  • 3:42 - 3:46
    So what makes a good representation of this sort?
  • 3:46 - 3:51
    Well, you should show the information that you need and nothing else.
  • 3:51 - 3:55
    And, what these representations should do is
  • 3:55 - 4:01
    it should enable to kinds of tasks that users want to do like comparison and exploration and problem solving.
  • 4:01 - 4:04
    And if that seems too abstract or maybe obvious,
  • 4:04 - 4:07
    let’s take a look at this example from the London Underground.
  • 4:07 - 4:10
    This subway map was introduced about a century ago
  • 4:10 - 4:17
    and it was one of the very first maps to introduce a brand-new idea in map design:
  • 4:17 - 4:25
    of abstracting the layout of the tracks from the underlying physical geography.
  • 4:25 - 4:30
    Prior to this London subway map, the maps would show what the geography was
  • 4:30 - 4:32
    and so long things were long and short things were short;
  • 4:32 - 4:35
    and if the tracks wandered because that’s the way that it works;
  • 4:35 - 4:39
    then the tracks on the map would wander because that’s the way things worked.
  • 4:39 - 4:43
    And with the Underground map designers realized was that
  • 4:43 - 4:50
    the most common task for subway riders is to figure out how to get from point A to point B,
  • 4:50 - 4:55
    and all of this additional detail of faithfulness to the underlying topology
  • 4:55 - 5:01
    was getting in the way of that A-to-B task more than it was helping it.
  • 5:01 - 5:05
    And so what they did is they stripped that a lot of that unnecessary detail,
  • 5:05 - 5:10
    turning this into vertical, diagonal, and horizontal lines.
  • 5:10 - 5:16
    So there are some representation between the layout on the map and the layout on the real world —
  • 5:16 - 5:18
    North is north, and south is south,
  • 5:18 - 5:22
    and things roughly head in the direction that they do in the real world,
  • 5:22 - 5:24
    but the detail is stripped out.
  • 5:24 - 5:30
    And this makes it much easier to be able to figure out how to get between connections.
  • 5:30 - 5:32
    Another thing that they did is they introduced
  • 5:32 - 5:38
    what a century later we would call a “focus plus context” representation for the map.
  • 5:38 - 5:42
    In the center of London, the subway stations are very densely packed.
  • 5:42 - 5:46
    So that area is expanded out: it consumes more of the map real estate.
  • 5:46 - 5:51
    As you get out toward the suburbs, the stations are fewer and further between.
  • 5:51 - 5:55
    As oppose to that taking up 90% of the map because it’s 90% of the space.
  • 5:55 - 5:58
    Those stations are actually scrunched,
  • 5:58 - 6:01
    because if you know you need to go northeast to a particular station,
  • 6:01 - 6:07
    then the exact distances involved are most of the time less relevant.
  • 6:07 - 6:11
    Now, of course with constitutes of good representation is of course task-specific.
  • 6:11 - 6:16
    And so what you can see is that by making some tasks easier —
  • 6:16 - 6:19
    like getting from A to B when you know A and B,
  • 6:19 - 6:24
    or being able to navigate the center of London more effectively —
  • 6:24 - 6:26
    you’ve compromised on other tasks.
  • 6:26 - 6:29
    And so, for somebody who may need to make decisions
  • 6:29 - 6:33
    about what stop to get off at based on some underlying topography,
  • 6:33 - 6:40
    or another task that’s compromised is by virtue of the distance between the stations on the map
  • 6:40 - 6:44
    not lining up between the distances between stations in the world,
  • 6:44 - 6:48
    you can make poor judgments about what’s close and what’s far:
  • 6:48 - 6:53
    In the center you may believe things are far apart when they are really close,
  • 6:53 - 6:58
    and out in the suburbs, you may believe from the map that things are closer than they actually are.
  • 6:58 - 7:04
    And so nearly all representation design is about fitness to task.
  • 7:04 - 7:07
    Here’s a temperature map from the Weather Underground.
  • 7:07 - 7:10
    It shows the temperature at each location in the Bay area,
  • 7:10 - 7:17
    geo-referenced so that the temperature number is placed right on top of that physical location.
  • 7:17 - 7:21
    What do you think are the benefits and drawbacks of this representation?
  • 7:21 - 7:26
    What’s it good for and what’s it a problem for?
  • 7:31 - 7:35
    If you know the physical coordinate that you’d like the temperature for,
  • 7:35 - 7:39
    say for example, “What temperature is it along the coast?”
  • 7:39 - 7:46
    and I don’t care or don’t know the exact name of the town, this works incredibly well.
  • 7:46 - 7:50
    It’s also a reasonable interface, in some sense,
  • 7:50 - 7:54
    for trying to get a good [inaudible] of what are the temperatures like in the area overall:
  • 7:54 - 8:00
    I can see, for example, as I head inland the temperature tends to get warmer.
  • 8:00 - 8:04
    There are a lot of ways in which we could make this better.
  • 8:04 - 8:10
    So, right now, every temperature is shown identically no matter what temperature it is
  • 8:10 - 8:15
    which means it’s hard to scan: I have to read every single temperature one by one.
  • 8:15 - 8:22
    I could make this better if instead I had the temperature number somehow —
  • 8:22 - 8:29
    the color or size of the temperature number — correspond to the weather,
  • 8:29 - 8:37
    But if we’re going to start mapping the variables of the map to something like color,
  • 8:37 - 8:39
    we need to be careful to get it right.
  • 8:39 - 8:43
    This is an example that comes from Edward Tufte.
  • 8:43 - 8:49
    His books on visual design and the graphical representations of data are fantastic
  • 8:49 - 8:51
    and I strongly encourage you to read them.
  • 8:51 - 8:59
    In this map, we see how a computer scientist might make a mapping for Japan.
  • 8:59 - 9:04
    This is showing the height above or below sea level as color,
  • 9:04 - 9:12
    and what you can see is the depth below sea level is represented by the color spectrum Roy G. Biv.
  • 9:12 - 9:21
    Now, one of the challenges of hue is that it’s not an additive representation.
  • 9:21 - 9:27
    So it’s not really a strong ordering that we give to the colors perceptually.
  • 9:27 - 9:33
    It’s a substitutive representation that red and yellow are qualitatively different,
  • 9:33 - 9:37
    but we don’t automatically have a more-than or less-than relationship between the two.
  • 9:37 - 9:39
    Another problem on this representation,
  • 9:39 - 9:45
    is it’s very difficult by glance to tell what’s higher and what’s lower in the sea.
  • 9:45 - 9:56
    Conversely, the individual isosurfaces — the individual chunks of a particular depth — really pop out.
  • 9:56 - 10:02
    Like, to me, for example, the yellow depth pops out very strongly
  • 10:02 - 10:06
    and that shape really comes to the attentional fore,
  • 10:06 - 10:11
    which, if you are making a rock-and-roll poster for the Fillmore, would be awesome,
  • 10:11 - 10:15
    but if you’re trying to get a sense of the contours of the sea,
  • 10:15 - 10:23
    what become salient to you, the outlines for what’s 400 meters below sea level, it’s probably just not that relevant.
  • 10:23 - 10:28
    So how could we continue our theme of using color as a representational cue
  • 10:28 - 10:33
    but have it be more meaningful than you might see in this case
  • 10:33 - 10:36
    where we’re mapping it to the color spectrum?
  • 10:36 - 10:40
    And here’s Edward Tufte’s redesign which I think is much better.
  • 10:40 - 10:44
    There’s a couple of things that I really like about the representation here.
  • 10:44 - 10:51
    The first one is that all of the things that are above sea level are brown — are kind of an earth tone.
  • 10:51 - 10:57
    So, we’re leveraging our intuitions about the physical world and using that metaphorically for the map.
  • 10:57 - 11:01
    So, the land-colored stuff is land.
  • 11:01 - 11:06
    Similarly, all of the water is blue — the water-colored stuff is water.
  • 11:06 - 11:16
    And furthermore, we can see how the intensity — or the luminance — of that color blue changes with depth.
  • 11:16 - 11:23
    And the deeper blues are darker blue, which corresponds to our physical intuitions.
  • 11:23 - 11:30
    And of course, water doesn’t really get that much darker at the kind of depth that we’re talking about here —
  • 11:30 - 11:35
    our intuition about darker colors being deeper comes from much shallower depths.
  • 11:35 - 11:41
    But the idea — you can leverage this thing that we all know that water right by the shore is a paler color
  • 11:41 - 11:44
    and as you get more of it gets darker.
  • 11:44 - 11:52
    So this is a really wonderful way to see that these darker areas here are deeper than these shallow areas here.
  • 11:52 - 11:58
    With the London Underground map, we saw how the representation of the map —
  • 11:58 - 12:04
    what makes a good representation — was intrinsically tied up with the task that the user is doing.
  • 12:04 - 12:12
    Similarly, what makes a good representation is also dependent on what a user’s expertise is.
  • 12:12 - 12:19
    And a wonderful example of this comes from Herb Simon and [Bill] Chase in the early 1970’s.
  • 12:19 - 12:25
    They were looking at chess as an exemplar domain for trying to understand expertise.
  • 12:25 - 12:28
    One of the things that they observed was that
  • 12:28 - 12:35
    expert chess players were much better at being able to remember the configuration of a chess board.
  • 12:35 - 12:39
    You can imagine a couple hypotheses for this.
  • 12:39 - 12:47
    So, one of them would be “Experts were born with a better memory for that kind of thing”;
  • 12:47 - 12:52
    Or, similarly, “Experts by virtue of their ten thousand hours of training
  • 12:52 - 12:57
    have trained themselves up to build up that muscle and be very good at remembering that kind of thing.”
  • 12:57 - 13:00
    Neither turns out to be the case.
  • 13:00 - 13:12
    Experts are much better at remembering the configuration of a board, but only if it’s an actual game!
  • 13:12 - 13:19
    So if the configuration of the chess board is the configuration of how a chess board could be,
  • 13:19 - 13:22
    experts do a fantastic job of remembering it.
  • 13:22 - 13:29
    But if you arranged the pieces on the board in a way that a chess play could not ever achieve,
  • 13:29 - 13:32
    the experts actually do no better than novices at all.
  • 13:32 - 13:41
    And so what we’re seeing is that the ability of experts to chunk things and have higher memory capacity
  • 13:41 - 13:47
    is because they are able to leverage their knowledge about the domain.
  • 13:47 - 13:52
    Game design and user interface design are both concerned
  • 13:52 - 13:56
    with how easy or hard it’s for a user to accomplish a particular task.
  • 13:56 - 14:01
    The difference is that often designers want to make it hard, the right hard;
  • 14:01 - 14:04
    and interface game designers want to make things easy.
  • 14:04 - 14:07
    And so we can learn from this chess example
  • 14:07 - 14:10
    and we can ask this question as interface designers:
  • 14:10 - 14:14
    “Can we make interfaces more chunkable?”
  • 14:14 - 14:18
    Can we make interactions that can be accomplished in one chunk
  • 14:18 - 14:24
    and therefore place a lower load on our memory and make it easier for users to work with?
  • 14:24 - 14:26
    A great example of this comes from Bill Buxton
  • 14:26 - 14:33
    who looked at being able to move text between locations on a document.
  • 14:33 - 14:37
    And in a common desktop user interface today,
  • 14:37 - 14:44
    one common operation would be either a keyboard shortcut or a menu command to cut some text,
  • 14:44 - 14:48
    and then you move the cursor to a new location, and you paste that text.
  • 14:48 - 14:54
    That’s three different operations and, in between, if you got interrupted,
  • 14:54 - 14:59
    you might forget what’s in the copy buffer — in fact, I’m sure that’s happened to all of us at some point.
  • 14:59 - 15:09
    What Buxton realized is what if you could turn all of this, through a gestural interface, into one command?
  • 15:09 - 15:17
    So, maybe I could grab a text that I want, draw a new location and drop it right there.
  • 15:17 - 15:23
    That’d be much better: There’s never a time where I could be interrupted and lose state
  • 15:23 - 15:28
    because all of the state is maintained in this continuous gesture.
  • 15:28 - 15:32
    We’ve all heard the saying that a picture is worth ten thousand words.
  • 15:32 - 15:40
    As interface designers, we’re tasked with the project of representing the information to the user
  • 15:40 - 15:45
    and one task that we commonly have to deal with is:
  • 15:45 - 15:51
    Should we represent information visually, or should we represent information textually?
  • 15:51 - 15:54
    The answer of course is it depends.
  • 15:54 - 16:00
    But one time when representing information visually can be much more effective
  • 16:00 - 16:06
    is when you can convert slow reasoning tasks into fast perception tasks
  • 16:06 - 16:10
    by virtue of making them visually salient.
  • 16:10 - 16:15
    We saw that with the map example: In that case, both the colorings of the map were visual,
  • 16:15 - 16:21
    but [in] one, it was much easier to just add a glance to understand what’s going on in good coloring.
  • 16:21 - 16:27
    And the poor coloring, you have to reason about it much more slowly and the wrong things kept popping out.
  • 16:27 - 16:31
    If you think about a table of numbers, it can often be difficult to see trends,
  • 16:31 - 16:35
    whereas if you represent that same information visually,
  • 16:35 - 16:40
    it can often pop out what the high points, the low points, trends, outliers —
  • 16:40 -
    all of that becomes salient and automatically visible to you.
Title:
Lecture 6.2: Distributing Cognition (Part 1/2) (16:44)
Video Language:
English

English subtitles

Revisions